Thinking, Fast and Slow

by

Daniel Kahneman

Thinking, Fast and Slow: Part 1, Chapter 1 Summary & Analysis

Summary
Analysis
Kahneman opens by allowing us to observe our minds in two different processing modes. He first provides an image of an angry-looking woman, eyebrows furrowed and mouth agape. He tells readers to note how they automatically observe her to be angry, perhaps about to say something loud and unkind. He says that this is an instance of “fast thinking.”
In order to demonstrate some of the faults in the way we think and process information, Kahneman must first introduce us to the two ways in which we do so. One of Kahneman’s main strategies for this, which he carries out through the book, is by asking us to observe our own brains at work.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Next, Kahneman instructs us to solve the problem 17 x 24. He states that we know we could probably solve the problem with effort, and perhaps with paper and pencil. He notes that when we solve it, we proceed through a sequence of steps, burdened by holding information in our heads. This is an example of slow thinking.
Like the example of the angry woman, Kahneman allows us to observe the limits of our “fast thinking” and show how “slow thinking” takes a lot more deliberate effort, as when we try to solve a more complex math problem.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman adopts the terms used by psychologists Keith Stanovich and Richard West, who referred to these two processes as “System 1” and “System 2.”  System 1 operates automatically and quickly. System 2 allocates attention to complex and effortful mental activities.
By introducing the two systems, Kahneman builds a framework that will eventually allow us to understand the limits of each individual kind of processing.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman then lists some examples of System 1 and System 2 processing: System 1 detects distance, orients to sounds, allows us to drive a car on an empty road, automatically answers 2 + 2 = ?, reads words, and understands simple sentences. System 1, Kahneman says, is responsible for “effortlessly originating impressions and feelings.” Kahneman also notes that many of these mental actions are completely involuntary.
Providing relatable and simple examples of System 1 processing goes a long way in allowing us to understand how much it encompasses. System 1 is vital to processing sensory information like sounds and sights and automatic calculations that our brain has developed over time.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Get the entire Thinking, Fast and Slow LitChart as a printable PDF.
Thinking, Fast and Slow PDF
System 2, on the other hand, is responsible for thoughts and actions that require attention, and which are disrupted if attention is drawn away. This includes focusing on a single voice in a crowded room, looking for a woman with white hair, counting the instances of the letter “a” on a page, comparing two washing machines for value, and checking the validity of a complex logical argument. These things do not come naturally and require exertion of at least some effort.
The list of processing responsibilities belonging to System 2 is varied, allowing Kahneman to demonstrate the way in which System 2 encompasses a wide array of actions and thoughts that require a bit more effort, distinguishing it from System 1.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman writes that the phrase “pay attention” is apt, because we have a limited budget of attention to allocate. We cannot calculate 17 x 24 when driving a car in heavy traffic—we must focus on one activity or the other.
Kahneman introduces this first pitfall of System 2, noting that we only have so much attention to allocate—and the way in which we divide our attention can sometimes lead to mistakes.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman illustrates this concept in a famous study conducted by Christopher Chabris and Daniel Simons. They showed participants a short film of two teams—one wearing white and one wearing black—passing basketballs. The viewers are instructed to count the passes of the white team and ignore the team with black shirts. Most people become so focused on the task, and about half of them do not notice a woman wearing a gorilla suit who appears, crosses the court, thumps her chest, and moves on. The gorilla study shows that we can be blind to the obvious, and blind to our blindness.
Kahneman goes on to describe a famous experiment that illustrates the mistakes of System 2. The experiment provides a salient—and funny—example of how difficult it is for System 2 to be attuned to more than one task at once. Even the most surprising events don’t faze us when we are deliberately focused on other things. The experiment provides a visual example of that concept, but throughout the book Kahneman shows it to be true of cognitive concepts as well.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Quotes
When System 1 runs into difficulty, it calls on System 2 to support it, as in the problem of 17 x 24. System 2 also comes into play when people experience a surprise that violates the expected model of the world (such as when they notice a person in a gorilla suit in a basketball game), because it then tries to make sense of the surprising stimulus. System 2 also continuously monitors a person’s behavior and works to maintain self-control.
The idea that System 2 tries to constantly interpret surprising events in the world is revisited in later chapters, when Kahneman demonstrates how we prefer to rely on narratives to make sense of the world around us, rather than try to understand that much of what happens in the world is due to randomness.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Stories and Subjectivity vs. Statistics and Objectivity Theme Icon
The division of labor between System 1 and System 2 is highly efficient, minimizing effort. System 1 is generally sufficient and its models of familiar situations are generally accurate. It does have biases, however, and it cannot be turned off.
The involuntary nature of System 1 will become one of its primary pitfalls, which Kahneman will discuss in greater detail when he brings up visual and cognitive illusions later in the chapter.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman then asks readers to participate in an experiment, reading two sets of words. In each set, there are two columns (a left and right column). Some of the words are in all upper-case letters, and some of them are in all lower-case letters. The difference between the two sets is that in the left set, the words alternate between “left” and “right.” In the right set, the words alternate between “upper” and “lower.” Kahneman asks people to complete two tasks: first calling out whether the words a person is reading is in upper or lower case, and then calling out whether the word is printed to the left of center or the right of center.
The task allows us to observe, as we try it ourselves, how we recognize that we have to change our processing. We consciously slow down as we complete the task so that we can overcome what Kahneman later calls a “cognitive minefield.” The goal of the book is to help us recognize other situations like this, in which we have to force our System 2 to overcome our System 1 processing.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
What Kahneman reveals, and what we find when we participate, is that it is easier to call out whether the word is in upper or lower case if we are not reading the words “upper” and “lower,” and that it is easier to call out whether the word is printed to the left or right if we are not reading the words “left” and “right.” It is harder to carry out the task when there is a conflict between that task and an automatic response (reading the word itself). System 2 is called in to overcome the impulses of System 1.
The reason that it is difficult to call out “upper” and “lower” when we are also reading those two words is because we are being primed for those words, a concept that Kahneman will elaborate on in Chapter 4. Priming adjusts our automatic processing because we already have those words in the forefront of our minds, and it is hard to overcome an automatic response like reading.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman next introduces illusions, including a famous image called the Müller-Lyer illusion. It shows two figures: on the top is a horizontal line with arrows or fins attached to it, that point outward, away from the line. On the bottom is another horizontal line with arrows or fins that point inward, towards the line. Even though measuring would reveal that the two horizontal lines are the same, the horizontal line in the bottom figure always appears longer. Thus, even though we believe that the two lines are equally long, that is not our impression of it.
The Müller-Lyer illusion allows us to recognize the difference between our impressions and our beliefs. We believe and know, after measuring, that the two lines are the same length, but we still have the impression that the bottom one is longer. This is another kind of flawed automatic thinking that Kahneman believes we have to learn to overcome.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
The Müller-Lyer illusion is an example of a visual illusion, but there are cognitive illusions as well. As a graduate student, Kahneman attended courses on psychotherapy. One professor said that the students might meet a patient who will share a tale of mistakes in his past treatment, but who feels that they (the students) will be able to help—a feeling the students would share. The professor then tells them that despite their sympathy and their intuition, they would not be able to help this patient, as he is likely a psychopath.
The difference between a visual and a cognitive illusion is that one can simply learn the correct answer in a visual illusion like Müller-Lyer. But for cognitive illusions, we are often unaware of them until someone points them out, like Kahneman’s professor here. This is why Kahneman wrote the book, so that we can become aware of and learn the mistakes we make due to cognitive illusions.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman understands that it is impractical to constantly question our own thinking. But he writes that what we can do is learn to “recognize situations in which mistakes are likely and try harder to avoid significant mistakes when the stakes are high.”
Even though cognitive illusions are harder to assess, as Kahneman writes, recognize the general principles that cause us to make cognitive mistakes will help us to see our blinds spots in the future.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Quotes
Kahneman then includes a disclaimer by saying that he talks about System 1 and System 2 as though they have personalities, abilities, limitations, and agency. He says that this way of speaking is considered taboo in his professional circles because they conjure up images of little people inside a person’s head. But Kahneman explains that he only uses the two terms as a shorthand, and using the two systems as the subjects of sentences makes it easier for people to understand those sentences.
It is important to recognize that these two systems are outgrowths of our consciousness, and not separate from us. But the reason Kahneman uses them as agents is also because, as he notes later, we have a preference for stories that make sense of the world, and building a narrative about two “characters” allows us to understand those concepts better.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Stories and Subjectivity vs. Statistics and Objectivity Theme Icon